6 Interestingly, activation levels of the superficial core muscle

6 Interestingly, activation levels of the superficial core muscles (lumbar multifidus, internal oblique, iliocostalis lumborum pars thoracis, external oblique, rectus abdominus, and erector spinae) were found to be similar between sittings on stable and unstable surfaces.6 and 11 It was speculated that profound core muscles may be more MAPK Inhibitor Library concentration active during active sitting.6 To date, biomechanical analyses

of active sitting were constrained to data obtained from 5 to 10 min sitting tests.6 and 11 As prolonged sitting was thought to inflict low-back conditions,2 it is important to examine the trunk biomechanics during active sitting over a longer time period (e.g., 30 min or more). Furthermore, the effect of active sitting on the pattern of foot center of pressure has been overlooked in the past. Although it was reported that sitting on an unstable surface results in increased spinal motion,6 it is not clear whether Volasertib molecular weight core muscles are exclusively used to modulate the trunk position. In a recent study, some leg muscles such as hip adductors, soleus, and tibialis anterior were found to increase their activity levels as the level

of sitting compliance increases.11 Thus, it may be possible that lower-extremities may partially contribute to the adjustment of the trunk posture during active sitting. However, it has yet to be determined whether lower extremities play a role in maintaining trunk posture during active sitting. In particular, the patterns of the foot center of pressure Non-specific serine/threonine protein kinase need to be examined. The primary purpose of this study was to determine if increased seating surface compliance would result in increased trunk motion during prolonged sitting. As the seating surface becomes unstable, there could be

an increase of the trunk motion. We hypothesized that the stability ball and air-cushion conditions would significantly increase trunk motion signified by increased trunk range of motion (T_ANG), trunk angular speed (T_AVEL), and trunk center of mass speed (T_COM), compared to the stable chair condition. The secondary purpose of this study was to examine whether lower-extremities are involved in active sitting. As seating surface compliance increases, it may be possible to have some contribution from the lower-legs to the adjustment of the trunk posture. Thus, we hypothesized that the unstable seating surfaces may lead to increases of foot center of pressure speed during sitting. Fifteen healthy females (age = 25.8 ± 10.3 years; height = 164.1 ± 7.1 cm; mass = 64.5 ± 12.8 kg) who sit for an average of 8 h per day volunteered for this study. Participants had a body mass index below 30 kg/m2 (23.8 ± 3.7 kg/m2), no known neuromuscular conditions, no history of low-back pain, and were able to sit for three 30-min sessions while maintaining upright posture. Each participant completed an informed consent document approved by the Ball State University Institutional Review Board.

The correlation between the average contrast sensitivity of bipol

The correlation between the average contrast sensitivity of bipolar cell synapses and the distribution of luminance sensitivities I1/2 is also shown in Figure S6B. This correlation can be understood in terms of the results in Figure 7: an individual terminal selleck compound is expected to exhibit its maximal contrast sensitivity at I1/2, so contrast sensitivity averaged across the whole population should parallel the distribution of I1/2 (Figure 5C). To compare the contrast sensitivities of linear and nonlinear terminals, we made measurements at five different mean luminances spanning 4 log units (Figure 8A). However,

for each terminal we only used responses to contrast measured at a mean luminance closest to its own value of I1/2. In ON terminals, the contrast generating the half-maximal response, C1/2, was 76% ± 8% in the linear group, and 54% ± 7% in the nonlinear group (Figure 8D). In OFF terminals, C1/2 was 75% ± 9% in the linear group, and 20% ± 4% in the nonlinear (Figure 8E). Thus, nonlinear OFF terminals were the most sensitive to temporal contrast. The modeling in Figures

7C and 7D explains this observation on the basis of nonlinear OFF terminals displaying the steepest luminance tuning curve, and this idea is supported by the results in Figures 8F and 8G: C1/2 was lowest (i.e., contrast-sensitivity highest) in nonlinear terminals with Hill coefficients greater than 1.5. Together, SP600125 in vitro the results in Figure 7 and Figure 8 demonstrate how a detailed description of the luminance tuning curve also helps us understand retinal signaling under natural conditions, when the visual stimulus involves fluctuations around

a recent mean. Imaging synaptic vesicle fusion has allowed us to make an in vivo survey of the visual signal as it is transmitted to the inner retina through the Adenosine population of bipolar cells. Two properties that varied across these synapses affected the transmission of information about the luminance and contrast of a visual stimulus. First, the luminance sensitivities of individual terminals varied across 4 log units, with a log-normal distribution similar to that observed in natural scenes. As a result, the sensitivity of synaptic transmission to a fluctuating stimulus depended on the mean luminance around which this fluctuation occurred relative to the luminance sensitivity of the terminal. Second, about half the synapses employed a triphasic tuning curve in which the largest deflection was a strongly supralinear function of luminance. These unusual tuning curves provided for a high degree of discriminability over a narrow range of luminances and an increased sensitivity to temporal contrast.

In contrast, higher neutralising capacity for the yellow fever vi

In contrast, higher neutralising capacity for the yellow fever virus in subjects with anti-dengue IgG antibodies has been reported, and hypothesised that subgroups with positive serology for dengue could develop cross-reactions with anti-yellow fever antibodies [16].

In 2013, the WHO Strategic Advisory Group of Experts (SAGE) announced that a single dose of the yellow fever vaccine provides life-long immunity and that revaccination every 10 years is not necessary for people who live in or travel to risk areas [4]. This new guideline was based on surveillance data indicating that vaccination failures are extremely rare and do not cluster as time increases after immunisations [4]. However, the known limitations in the surveillance of yellow fever cases and in the management of vaccination records, particularly in adults, suggest that data on vaccination

selleckchem failure are underestimated [14]. The rarity of vaccination failure could also be partly explained by the revaccination requirement in immunisation programmes and prior to travel to endemic areas. However, the absence of yellow fever cases in vaccinated travellers DNA Damage inhibitor does not appear to be a good indicator of the duration of immunity, considering that potential natural exposures, which warrant recommendation for vaccination, can impair the assessment of the long-term effects of vaccination. WHO’s recent recommendations have also generated controversies because the serological methods used have varied over the many decades during Sitaxentan which the studies that served as the basis for the recommendations

were performed [14]. In addition, the PRNT method that determines neutralising antibody titres, which is considered the best available measure of seroprotection following vaccination, has exhibited considerable heterogeneity and allows only limited comparability between results [14]. A review exploring the scientific evidence for a change in the vaccination recommendation proposed by the WHO [7] appears to disregard the possibility that seronegative subjects may have been a result of primary or secondary failures of the vaccine. In fact, the high levels of vaccine immunogenicity in clinical studies under controlled immunisation conditions in selected groups may not be reproduced in routine immunisation programmes. These are generally affected by problems related to vaccine conservation and application, and may include subjects with clinical complications that could compromise their immune response. Accordingly, the rate of seroconversion following routine vaccination in military personnel in this study has been reported to be slightly lower than that in healthy volunteers in controlled studies [15]. In addition, a weaker immune response can result in shorter immunity duration. Cut-off values correlating with protection are not available for antibody titres measured by serum-dilution plaque-reduction tests.

, 2009, Seo et al , 2009 and Sharma et al , 2008) Mutations in A

, 2009, Seo et al., 2009 and Sharma et al., 2008). Mutations in ACIII are associated with obesity in both mice and humans ( Nordman et al., 2008 and Wang et al., 2009b). Direct evidence of an association between cilia dysfunction and obesity comes from deleting Kif3a or Ift88 conditionally in adult mice. Cilia are stunted and animals

overeat, becoming obese. This occurs despite elevated leptin, a satiety signal, suggesting the satiety response is compromised in the absence of functional JQ1 solubility dmso cilia ( Davenport et al., 2007). Ciliated neurons that regulate feeding include a group of pro-opiomelanocortin (POMC)-expressing neurons in the hypothalamic arcuate nucleus. These cells respond to leptin by cleaving POMC to generate α-melanocyte-stimulating hormone (α-MSH), which inhibits feeding, and mice in which Kif3a is check details deleted selectively from POMC neurons become obese ( Davenport et al., 2007 and Sharma et al., 2008). What ciliary signaling pathways in POMC neurons might modulate α-MSH production, however, remains unclear. The localization of leptin receptors to POMC cell cilia might be predicted, for

example, but has not been confirmed. Connecting Shh signaling with the cilium illuminates CNS defects found in cilliopathic syndromes and other human disease states. Ciliated cerebellar granule neuron precursors (GNPs) proliferate in a Shh-dependent manner in the external granule layer (EGL) of the developing cerebellum (Dahmane and Ruiz i Altaba, 1999, Wallace, 1999 and Wechsler-Reya and Scott, 1999). In mice with conditional deletions of Ift88 or Kif3a, cilia on EGL precursors are stunted, cerebellar granule neurons are fewer, and the cerebellum is hypoplastic, similar to its appearance in Joubert

DNA ligase Syndrome ( Chizhikov et al., 2007 and Spassky et al., 2008). Learning disabilities are also a feature of several ciliopathies. Although associated defects in the hippocampus have yet to be reported in human patients, the hippocampal dentate gyrus (DG) in mice is highly sensitive to the disruption of primary cilia. DG progenitor cells are ciliated, and respond to Shh to generate granule neurons (Breunig et al., 2008 and Han et al., 2008). Deleting Kif3a in DG progenitor cells stunts primary cilia, decreases Shh signaling in the DG, and inhibits the normal perinatal expansion of DG progenitor population. This reduces the main wave of production of DG neurons in the first three weeks after birth, as well as the initial allocation of radial astrocyte precursors that supply new DG neurons into adulthood ( Han et al., 2008). Similar results are seen in mice deficient in Ift88 ( Han et al., 2008) and in the stumpy mouse mutant, in which ciliogenesis is still more impaired ( Breunig et al., 2008 and Town et al., 2008). The adult neural stem cells of the DG are themselves ciliated, and the role of primary cilia in these cells awaits analysis of mice with deletion of cilia in adulthood.

, 1995 and Song et al , 1998) The N-terminal regions of Munc13-1

, 1995 and Song et al., 1998). The N-terminal regions of Munc13-1 and ubMunc13-2 contain a Ca2+-independent C2A domain and a long sequence of unknown significance, followed by a central calmodulin-binding sequence and C1-domain. In contrast, bMunc13-2 and Munc13-3 have

a different, even longer N-terminal region upstream of the C1 domain (Figure 2). The short Munc13 isoforms (Munc13-4 and BAP3), conversely, lack all domains upstream of the C2B domain, placing the C2B domain at their N terminus. In all Munc13 isoforms, the C2B domain is followed by a large domain called the MUN domain and a C-terminal Ca2+-independent C2C domain (Figure 2). Munc13 proteins have two principal functions at the active zone:

to prime the SNARE/SM protein fusion machinery for exocytosis, thus rendering synaptic vesicles fusion competent, and to mediate short-term XAV939 Selleckchem PLX 4720 plasticity by regulating this priming activity. Munc13s execute their priming function via the MUN domain (Basu et al., 2005 and Stevens et al., 2005). The MUN domain may act to open the ‘closed’ form of the SNARE protein syntaxin-1 (Gerber et al., 2008), thereby enabling syntaxin-1 to form SNARE complexes (Richmond et al., 2001 and Ma et al., 2011). This function of the MUN domain may be general for regulated exocytosis, since Munc13-4 is essential for cytotoxic granule exocytosis in NK cells (Feldmann et al., 2003). Remarkably, a recent crystal structure of a fragment of the MUN domain has revealed similarities of the MUN domain to tethering factors involved in other intracellular trafficking steps, suggesting that the MUN domain may exert a conserved function similar to those of other tethering factors (Li et al., 2011). Moreover, a distantly related protein called CAPS that also

has a MUN domain is essential for dense-core vesicle priming for exocytosis and again functions by binding to SNARE/SM protein Idoxuridine complexes (Khodthong et al., 2011). How precisely the MUN interacts with SNARE and SM proteins, however, remains unclear. Deletion of all large Munc13 isoforms blocks synaptic vesicle exocytosis in autapses formed by hippocampal neurons but produces only a partial impairment of exocytosis in neuromuscular junction synapses (Varoqueaux et al., 2002 and Varoqueaux et al., 2005). Thus, it may be that different types of synapses exhibit distinct requirements for Munc13. Based on knockout studies, CAPS has also been suggested to function at synapses (Jockusch et al., 2007), but its role in synaptic exocytosis may be indirect. At present, it is unclear whether Munc13 and CAPS are functionally redundant, and whether Munc13 proteins are generally required for all regulated exocytosis similar to SNARE and SM proteins. Munc13′s are regulated at many levels. Their N-terminal C2A domain homodimerizes (Dulubova et al.

, 2009 and Papadia et al , 2008) We next studied whether express

, 2009 and Papadia et al., 2008). We next studied whether expression of GluN2BWT or GluN2B2A(CTR) had different effects on vulnerability to excitotoxicity. NMDA (20 μM) was applied for 1 hr to neurons transfected with vectors encoding either GluN2BWT, GluN2B2A(CTR) or control vector, and neuronal death was assessed 24 hr later. GluN2BWT strongly increased the level of cell death compared to the control, consistent with NMDAR currents being higher (Figures 1D and 1E). However, expression of GluN2B2A(CTR) caused a significantly lower enhancement

of cell death than GluN2BWT (Figures 1D and 1E), despite NMDAR currents being equal (Figure 1B), suggesting that CTD2B promotes cell death learn more better than CTD2A. The same result was found when the experiment was repeated in DIV18 neurons (see Figure S1A available online), indicating that the differential effect of CTD2B versus CTD2A on cell death also holds true in more mature neurons. To further investigate the differential CTD subtype effects on excitotoxicity, we compared NMDAR-dependent cell death in neurons expressing

GluN2AWT and GluN2A2B(CTR). Expression of GluN2AWT and GluN2A2B(CTR) did not differentially affect the proportion of extrasynaptic NMDARs (Figure 1C) and selleckchem caused similar increases in NMDAR currents (Figure 1F); although, because of the lower affinity of GluN2A for NMDA, the increases were smaller than for the GluN2B-based constructs (Figure 1B). We found that neurons expressing GluN2A2B(CTR) were significantly more vulnerable to NMDA-induced excitotoxicity than GluN2AWT-expressing neurons (Figure 1G). Thus, for a given amount of NMDAR-mediated current, the presence of CTD2B promotes neuronal death better than CTD2A, regardless of whether they are linked to the channel portion of GluN2A or GluN2B. This result illustrates

the independent influence of the identity of the CTD on excitotoxicity, acting in addition to the influence of the identity of the rest of the channel on downstream signaling Rolziracetam events (e.g., because of different channel kinetics and ligand binding properties). We next investigated the importance of the GluN2 CTD subtype by an independent approach: a genetically modified “knockin” mouse in which the protein coding portion of the C-terminal exon of GluN2B (encoding over 95% of the CTD) was exchanged for that of GluN2A (GluN2B2A(CTR); Figure 2A; see Supplemental Experimental Procedures). The 3′UTR of GluN2B, which also forms part of the C-terminal exon, was unchanged apart from a 61 bp insertion at its beginning (a remnant of the excision of a neomycin resistance selection cassette).

9, p < < 0 001, Spearman rank correlation, Figure S2A) Important

9, p < < 0.001, Spearman rank correlation, Figure S2A). Importantly, these observations held true when correlations were examined for

individual animals (Figure S2B). Thus, the overall reduction in correlated noise among MSTd neurons was a robust finding in trained animals. It is possible that the difference in correlated noise between naive and trained animals could be an indirect effect of training on the response properties of individual neurons. Moreover, training-related changes in correlated noise might emerge in parallel with changes in the heading sensitivity of single neurons. To address these issues, we examined the effect of training on the time courses of firing rates and response variability. As illustrated in Figures 3A and 3C, the time course of the population-average response to the preferred heading was indistinguishable between trained and naive animals (p = 0.8, permutation test, C59 see Experimental Procedures). There was also no significant effect (p = 0.5, permutation test) of training on the time course of the Fano factor, which measures the ratio of response variance to mean response (Figures 3B and 3D, see also Experimental Procedures and Figure S3). This finding contrasts with a previous report that Fano factor in area V4 was significantly reduced after animals were trained to discriminate orientation (Raiguel et al., 2006). In

MSTd, the difference in noise correlation between naive and trained animals does not appear to be linked to changes in firing rates or Fano

factors. We further explored whether training shaped the tuning properties of individual MSTd neurons. Selleck MAPK Inhibitor Library For this analysis, we only included neurons with significant heading tuning in the horizontal plane (p < 0.05, one-way ANOVA). To gain statistical power, we exploited a much larger database of single-unit responses from naive and trained animals, recorded with a single electrode (vestibular: n = 556; visual: n = 992). As shown in Figures 4A and 4E, distributions of tuning width (full width at half-height) were very similar for naive and trained animals. There was no significant difference in median tuning width for the visual condition (naive: 124.5° versus trained: 126°, p = 0.21, Wilcoxon Histone demethylase rank- sum test). The difference in median tuning width was significant for the vestibular condition (naive: 121° versus trained: 131°, p = 0.045). However, this effect was weak and, notably, training slightly increased tuning width in the vestibular condition, an effect opposite to that expected if training increases discriminability (e.g., Yang and Maunsell, 2004). Similarly, as shown in Figures 4B and 4F, training did not have any significant effect on the distribution of tuning curve amplitudes in either the visual condition (naive: 35.4 spks/s versus trained: 31.8 spks/s, p = 0.24, Wilcoxon rank-sum test) or the vestibular condition (naive: 17.4 spks/s versus trained: 17.2 spks/s, p = 0.36).

The average regression estimate was negative, indicating greater

The average regression estimate was negative, indicating greater firing selleck on trials that started with the rat near the lever. We performed additional analyses to confirm and test the specificity of these results. First, to verify

the jointly obtained GLM estimates, we used a sequential estimation procedure to find the semipartial correlation coefficients between each variable and firing rate. The results did not differ from the GLM results in any meaningful way (Table S3). Second, we found that locomotor and proximity encoding was evident very shortly after cue onset (50–200 ms) but not prior to cue onset (−1,000 to 0 ms), indicating that movement encoding arose rapidly at cue onset, at a short enough time scale to influence movement even on trials with a short

locomotor onset latency. Finally, we repeated the GLM analysis in non-cue-excited neurons and found little encoding of locomotion or lever proximity. See Supplemental Information for details. The comprehensive regression model results suggests that the cue-evoked firing encodes the vigor of locomotion (onset latency and speed), whereas the lack of significant relationship with mean angular velocity, which is related to turn direction, suggests no encoding of the direction of locomotion. However, this hypothesis is only partially tested by the 16-term model used above because the regressors representing movement latency and turn direction, chosen based on the PCA/FA results, are indirect measures that are only correlated with these parameters (Table S2). Another potential concern with find protocol the 16-term model is the possibility of overfitting due to the large number of independent variables relative to the number of trials. Therefore, to explicitly test the hypothesis that cue-evoked firing encodes vigor but not turn direction, we employed

a focused GLM using only four regressors: the direct measurement of movement onset latency; the direct measurement of turn direction, where positive values indicate turns contralateral to the recorded neuron; average speed; and proximity to the lever at cue onset, a highly significant regressor in the first analysis. We performed the focused GLM on the 57 cue-excited neurons for which sufficient data were Linifanib (ABT-869) available. The results confirmed that these neurons fired more on trials with short movement onset latency (Figure 4A), with fast movement speed (Figure 4B), and that started near the lever (Figure 4D), but showed no overall firing bias for contralateral or ipsilateral movement direction (Figure 4C). Notably, 11 of 57 cue-excited neurons had individually significant encoding of proximity to the lever (Figure 4D). Using these regression estimates, we observed a weak negative correlation between speed encoding and latency encoding (Figure 4E); that is, neurons with more firing on trials with fast speed (positive effect of speed) also tended to show more firing on trials with short movement latency (negative effect of movement onset).

Original recordings were then decimated (averaged 10 points, 1 ms

Original recordings were then decimated (averaged 10 points, 1 ms). Single peak spontaneous IPSCs with amplitudes greater than 2.1 times the SD of baseline noise were detected using a semiautomated sliding template detection procedure with AxoGraph X. The template was generated by averaging

multiple sIPSCs. Each detected event was visually inspected. Events were discarded if the average baseline noise (>300 ms) was greater than the peak ±1.5 s from the peak. Peak amplitudes were determined by averaging the current ±20 ms see more from the greatest upward deflection. The amplitude distribution of the baseline noise was measured by averaging the baseline current ±20 ms from the greatest upward deflection, 220 ms after a point set to 0 pA, once every 50 s (n = 26 cells). To compare the kinetics of eIPSCs to sIPSCs, spontaneous events with a single peak were selected. Duration of eIPSCs and sIPSCs was determined by measuring the width at 20% of the peak amplitude (see Figure 1C). All drugs were applied through

bath perfusion, except dopamine, which was applied by iontophoresis. Iontophoretic pipettes (70–110 MΩ) were filled with 1 M dopamine and the tip placed within 10 μm of the soma. A negative backing current (6–11 nA) prevented leakage. Dopamine was ejected with the application of positive current (2–6 s) with an Axoclamp 2A amplifier to elicit a maximal dopamine-induced outward current. A transgenic mouse expressing the human D2 dopamine receptor short isoform (hD2S) with a flag DNA Damage inhibitor epitope on the amino terminus was generated by nuclear microinjection using standard techniques. The transgene consisted of an 8.5 kb genomic fragment from the rat tyrosine hydroxylase gene (TH) containing 5′ regulatory sequences, the basal promoter, and 26 base pairs from the 5′ untranslated region in exon 1 followed by a 0.7 kb cassette containing intron 2 and splice donor/acceptor sites from the rabbit beta-globin gene (Arttamangkul et al., 2008). The hD2S construct consisted of a consensus Kozak sequence, a signal peptide from the hemagglutinin

influenza followed by the sequence for the FLAG epitope (Vickery and von Zastrow, 1999), a full-length cDNA for the hD2S containing 1.4 kb of coding Edoxaban sequence and 1.0 kb of 3′ untranslated, and the bovine growth hormone polyA sequence from pcDNA 3.0 (Invitrogen). After cleavage by the signal peptidase of the signal sequence during translation, an hD2S protein with an amino terminus Flag epitope is expressed in TH-expressing neurons including the dopamine neurons of the midbrain, as shown by immunostaining with the M1 anti-Flag antibody (Sigma-Aldrich) using confocal microscopy on sections and two-photon microscopy on midbrain slice preparations (data not shown). CGP 35348 and 5-CT were obtained from Tocris Bioscience. Baclofen and sulpiride were obtained from Research Biochemical. MK-801 was obtained from Abcam.

Embryos were placed back

Embryos were placed back Palbociclib supplier into the abdominal cavity, and mice were sutured and placed on a heating plate until recovery. Retroviral infections were performed using

a preparation of Moloney murine leukemia retrovirus expressing GFP under control of the CAG promoter (Jessberger et al., 2007). Lateral ventricles of E13.5 embryos were injected following the same protocol as for in utero electroporation. In utero infection of cortical progenitors was performed with a limiting dilution of GFP retrovirus in WT and KO mice at E13.5 (Jagasia et al., 2009). In these conditions, we consistently labeled a few isolated neurons and a couple of small groups of neurons, clearly isolated from each other (average number of cells per clones = 4.7; average number of clones per animal analyzed = 3.4; not found in the same section for instance or separated by at least 300 μm in depth). We focused our analysis on the isolated groups of radially oriented neurons in the somatosensory cortex that consisted of clonally related cells (Figure S4). Embryonic brains were electroporated at E14.5,

Protease Inhibitor Library and 300 μm embryonic brain slices were prepared at E15.5 using a Leica VT1000S vibrosector. Slices were cultured on confocal inserts (Millipore; 5 mm height) with 1.2 ml of Hank’s balanced salt solution (HBSS) supplemented with Eagle’s basal medium Oxymatrine and 5% of horse serum (Invitrogen). Time lapse confocal microscopy was performed using an LD Plan Neofluar 20×/0,40 with a Zeiss LSM 510 inverted

microscope by imaging multiple z stacks at preselected positions on a given set of electroporated slices. Repetitive acquisitions were performed every 20 min for up to 20 hr, and movies were assembled with Zeiss Zen imaging software. Thirty neurons per slice were randomly selected within the SVZ/IZ and tracked using the Manual Tracking plugin of ImageJ software. The position of each individual neuron was manually marked as the center of the neuron and recorded as XY coordinate on each image of 1,024 × 1,024 pixels. The length of the migration between consecutive time frames was calculated by converting the length of the XY vector formed between to time frames in microns (1.024 pixels = 300 μm). Following ephrin-B1 overexpression, a neuron was considered as “clustered” if it belongs to a group of at least five cells in contact with each other and “single” if its position is more than 10 μm away from a cluster at the end of the time lapse acquisition. The parameters of migration were calculated using homemade calculation software and Microsoft Excel 2011. Embryos were fixed by transcardiac perfusion with 4% paraformaldehyde (Invitrogen). Brains were dissected, and 100 μm sections were prepared using a Leica VT1000S vibrosector. Slices were transferred into PBS/0.